Skip to ContentSkip to Navigation
Part of University of Groningen
Science LinX Science Linx News

Darting around with a tiny brain

Why insects navigate more efficiently than robots
12 February 2024
Elisabetta Chicca and Thorben Schoepe with a robot they designed to test their model of insect's navigation. Photo Marleen Annema

With a brain the size of a pinhead, insects perform fantastic navigational feats. They avoid obstacles and move through small openings. How do they do this, with their limited brain power? Understanding the inner workings of an insect’s brain can help us in our search towards energy-efficient computing, physicist Elisabetta Chicca of the University of Groningen demonstrates with her most recent result: a robot that acts like an insect.

FSE Science Newsroom | Charlotte Vlek

Portrait of Elisabetta Chicca
Elisabetta Chicca. Photo Leoni von Ristok

It’s not easy to make use of the images that come in through your eyes, when deciding what your feet or wings should do. A key aspect here is the apparent motion of things as you move. ‘Like when you’re on a train’, Chicca explains. ‘The trees nearby appear to move faster than the houses far away. Insects use this information to infer how far away things are. This works well when moving in a straight line, but reality is not that simple.

Moving in curves makes the problem too complex for insects. To keep things manageable for their limited brainpower, they adjust their behaviour: they fly in a straight line, make a turn, then make another straight line. Chicca explains: ‘What we learn from this is: if you don’t have enough resources, you can simplify the problem with your behaviour.’

Brains on wheels

In search of the neural mechanism that drives insect behaviour, PhD student Thorben Schoepe developed a model of its neuronal activity and a small robot that uses this model to navigate. All this was done under Chicca’s supervision, and in close collaboration with neurobiologist Martin Egelhaaf of Bielefeld University, who helped to identify the insects’ computational principles.

(Text continues below picture)

decChicca and Schoepe with their robotorative image
Thorben Schoepe's robot in a corridor with random print. Photo Leoni von Ristok

Schoepe’s model is based on one main principle: always steer towards the area with the least apparent motion. He had his robot drive through a long ‘corridor'—consisting of two sides with a random print on it—and the robot centred in the middle of the corridor, as insects tend to do.

In other (virtual) environments, such as a space with obstacles or small openings, Schoepe’s model also showed similar behaviour to insects. ‘The model is so good’, Chicca concludes, ‘that once you set it up, it will perform in all kinds of environments. That’s the beauty of this result.’

Hardwired instead of learned

The fact that a robot can navigate in a realistic environment is not new. Rather, the model gives insight into how insects do the job, and how they manage to do things so efficiently. Chicca explains: ‘Much of Robotics is not concerned with efficiency. We humans tend to learn new tasks as we grow up and within Robotics, this is reflected in the current trend of machine learning. But insects are able to fly immediately from birth. An efficient way of doing that is hardwired in their brains.’

A small chip
A chip made by Chicca's group. Photo Leoni von Ristok

In a similar way, you could make computers more efficient. Chicca shows a chip that her research group has previously developed: a strip with a surface area as small as a key on your keyboard. In the future, she hopes to incorporate this specific insect behaviour in a chip as well. She comments: ‘Instead of using a general-purpose computer with all its possibilities, you can build specific hardware; a tiny chip that does the job, keeping things much smaller and energy-efficient.’

Details:

Schoepe, T., Janotte, E., Milde, M.B. et al. Finding the gap: neuromorphic motion-vision in dense environments. Nature Communications, 2024.

Elisabetta Chicca is part of the Groningen Cognitive Systems and Materials Center (CogniGron). Its mission is to develop self-learning materials and systems, inspired by the human brain, and thus to create a blueprint for future-proof computing.

Last modified:15 February 2024 10.27 a.m.
View this page in: Nederlands

More news

  • 24 May 2024

    Lustrum 410 in pictures

    Lustrum 410 in pictures: A photo report of the lustrum 2024

  • 21 May 2024

    Results of 2024 University elections

    The votes have been counted and the results of the University elections are in!

  • 13 May 2024

    Trapping molecules

    In his laboratory, physicist Steven Hoekstra is building an experimental set-up made of two parts: one that produces barium fluoride molecules, and a second part that traps the molecules and brings them to an almost complete standstill so they can...